- Micro Report
- Open access
- Published:
Targeted NMDA receptor knockdown in recall‐activated neuronal ensembles impairs remote fear extinction
Molecular Brain volume 18, Article number: 30 (2025)
Abstract
Fear extinction training in rodents decreases fear responses, providing a model for the development of post-traumatic stress disorder therapeutics. Fear memory recall reactivates the consolidated fear memory trace across multiple brain regions, and several studies have suggested that these recall-activated neurons are re-engaged during extinction. However, the molecular mechanisms linking this reactivation to extinction remain largely elusive. Here, we investigated the role of N-Methyl-d-Aspartate receptors (NMDARs) in remote memory recall–activated neurons within the basolateral amygdala and the medial prefrontal cortex during extinction training in mice. We found that Grin1 knockdown in these specific ensembles impaired extinction of remote fear memory, but did not reduce their reactivation during retrieval of the extinguished memory. These data suggest that while reactivation of these neuronal populations persists, their NMDARs are crucial for driving the synaptic plasticity needed to extinguish remote fear memories.
Main text
Extinction of obsolete fear memories is essential for effective adaptation to new environments. Rodent studies have provided both correlational [1] and causal [2, 3] evidence that neuronal activity in the infralimbic region of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and the basolateral amygdala (BLA) is crucial for successful extinction training. Furthermore, the activities of reciprocal connections between these regions underlie the extinction [4].
Recently, the reactivation of remote recall-activated neuronal ensembles has been suggested as a cellular mechanism of remote fear memory extinction [5]. However, the molecular mechanisms by which these reactivated neurons contribute to the attenuation of fear memory remain elusive. Given the pivotal role of N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors (NMDARs) in the BLA and the mPFC in extinction [6, 7], we hypothesized that NMDARs of the recall-activated neuronal ensembles function as a critical mediator of fear memory extinction.
To test whether NMDARs in remote recall-activated neurons in the BLA and the mPFC are necessary for the extinction training, we induced knockdown (KD) of NMDARs in these neurons by combining an activity-dependent labeling system with a virally delivered cre-dependent CRISPR-Cas9 system. A cocktail of AAVs encoding cre-dependent tdTomato and SaCas9 cassette was bilaterally infused to the BLA and the mPFC of Arc-CreERT2 mice. The SaCas9 cassette contained either sgRNA targeting exon 1 of the Grin1 gene (sgGrin1) or control sgRNA (3-base pair mismatch at the 3′ end, sgGrin1 ATG) (Supplementary Fig. 1 A). In a parallel experiment, we confirmed that both CaMKII-driven SaCas9 expression delivered via the AAV system and SpCas9 expression in a knock-in mouse significantly decreased the NMDA/AMPA ratio (Supplementary Fig. 1B–E).
Two weeks after auditory fear conditioning, mice underwent remote recall (Retrieval 1) of auditory fear memory during which 4-hydroxytamoxifen (4-OHT; 50 mg/kg) was administered to capture recall-activated ensembles for the Grin1 KD via CRISPR-Cas9 system (Fig. 1A). Four weeks later, mice were subjected to 2 days of extinction training and the next day, mice were sacrificed after a memory test (Retrieval 2) for post hoc electrophysiological analysis and immunohistology. In the BLA, tdTomato-expressing (tdT+) neurons from the sgGrin1 group exhibited significantly decreased levels of NMDAR-mediated currents compared to counterparts from the sgGrin1 ATG group indicating successful KD of NMDARs in recall-activated neurons (Fig. 1B). Immunohistochemical analysis revealed the significant colocalization of the Cas9 and the tdTomato proteins, and decreased level of GluN1 in tdT + neurons compared to surrounding tdT- neurons, further validating the specificity of our KD system (Supplementary Fig. 2 A–D).
NMDAR KD in the recall-activated ensembles in the BLA and the mPFC. A Behavioral experiment scheme. 4-OHT was delivered two hours before remote retrieval 1, to tag ensembles activated during retrieval 1. B Left, a representative image of the recorded slice. Middle, sgGrin1 group showed a significantly lower level of NMDA/AMPA ratio than the sgGrin1 ATG group. Each dot represents a cell; grey, control group injected with scrambled sgRNA (n = 6 cells); orange, test group injected with sgGrin1 (n = 7 cells). Right, a representative EPSC trace of each group. Mann–Whitney test, *p = 0.0350. C Freezing level during retrieval 1. Unpaired t-test, ns, p = 0.7884. D Retention of fear memory after 4-OHT induction measured by the first tone block of the extinction curve. sgGrin1 ATG group, N = 13; sgGrin1 group, N = 11. Unpaired t-test, ns, p = 0.6485. E Trace of the freezing level during the extinction day 1 and 2. Each tone block represents the average freezing level of consecutive two tones. sgGrin1 ATG group, N = 13; sgGrin1 group, N = 11. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA; time effect, p = 0.0923; Grin1 KO effect, **p = 0.0068; F Extinction index of the sgGrin1 group and the sgGrin1 ATG groups. sgGrin1 ATG group, N = 13; sgGrin1 group, N = 11. Mann–Whitney test, *p = 0.0410. G Representative image of c-fos immunohistochemistry analysis. H Left panel, the proportion of tdT + neurons among DAPI + cells in the BLA. Each dot represents the average value of images from an individual mouse. sgGrin1 ATG group, N = 9; sgGrin1 group, N = 7; Mann–Whitney test, ns, p = 0.9182; right panel, Reactivation/chance = P(cfos|tdTomato)/P(cfos|DAPI); Mann–Whitney test, ns, p = 0.2105; One-sample Wilcoxon test, #p = 0.0156, ##p = 0.0039. I Left panel, the proportion of tdT + neurons among DAPI + cells in the mPFC. Each dot represents the average value of images from an individual mouse. sgGrin1 ATG group, N = 8; sgGrin1 group, N = 7; Mann–Whitney test, ns, p = 0.1893; right panel, Reactivation/chance = P(cfos|tdTomato)/P(cfos|DAPI); Mann–Whitney test, ns, p = 0.3357; One-sample Wilcoxon test, #p = 0.0156, ##p = 0.0078. Data are presented as mean ± SEM
The freezing levels were comparable between the sgGrin1 and the control group during Retrieval 1 and four weeks after Retrieval 1, indicating that memory retention was not impaired by Grin1 KD (Fig. 1C, D). However, the Grin1 KD group showed a significantly disrupted extinction curve (Fig. 1E) and an abolished effect of extinction at Retrieval 2 (Fig. 1F). Nevertheless, tdT + neurons in both the mPFC and the BLA remained significantly responsive compared to chance levels, regardless of Grin1 KD (Fig. 1G–I).
In this study, we found that NMDARs in remote recall-activated ensembles within the BLA and the mPFC are crucial for the extinction of remote fear memory. Notably, despite the Grin1 KD, these neuronal ensembles in the BLA and the mPFC remained responsive to the CS during the recall after extinction.
It was hypothesized that extinction-induced reactivation of previous memory traces may provide a window for updating maladaptive fear memories [8]. In line with this notion, reactivation of neuronal ensembles in the dentate gyrus tagged during remote memory recall was necessary for extinction [5]. However, the molecular mechanisms linking such reactivation to the process of fear extinction remain unclear.
Previous studies utilizing region-specific conditional KD [9] or infusion of NMDAR antagonists [3, 10] demonstrated the critical role of NMDARs in extinction. While NMDAR blockade in the BLA impaired within-session extinction, the same approach in the infralimbic cortex (IL) led to the failure of extinction memory consolidation. In particular, the effectiveness of extinction training was correlated with NMDAR-dependent burst firing in IL neurons [10, 11] and extinction training-induced plasticity of NMDAR-mediated currents at ventral hippocampal synapses in the IL [12]. However, these earlier approaches lacked the cellular specificity needed to pinpoint how particular neuronal ensembles contribute to extinction learning. By combining activity-dependent tagging with target gene KD, we were able to investigate the critical role of NMDARs specifically within the remote recall-activated ensembles of the BLA and the mPFC.
Our results align with and extend previous findings in several key ways. First, the Grin1 KD in the recall-activated neurons in the BLA and the mPFC did not affect memory retention. To note, prolonged knockout of Grin1 in the forebrain significantly impaired remote fear memory retention [9]. This discrepancy may stem from the brain-wide nature of the memory trace [13], allowing other regions to compensate for local Grin1 KD in the BLA and mPFC. Second, we found a significantly impaired extinction of remote fear memory by Grin1 KD in recall-activated neurons. Indeed, earlier studies showed that the blockade of NMDARs in the mPFC [10] or BLA [7] impaired fear memory extinction. Our findings further suggest that the essential NMDARs identified in those experiments may specifically reside in the recall-activated ensembles.
Our results indicate that NMDARs in recall-activated neurons are required for fear memory extinction but are not essential for their reactivation during fear memory recall. Therefore, it is unlikely that NMDARs in these neurons mediate the unlearning of the fear memory trace through an LTD-like process at their synaptic inputs [14]. Instead, they may facilitate new learning of safety signals that suppress the previously established fear memory [15]. One possibility is that NMDARs enhance burst firing in recall-activated neurons, thereby promoting synaptic plasticity in downstream regions when combined with convergent inputs [10].
In summary, by incorporating the activity-dependent tagging system and the AAV-delivered CRISPR-Cas9 system, we showed that NMDARs in recall-activated ensembles in the BLA and mPFC are required for the extinction of fear memory. These findings provide important insight into how recall of fear memory is linked to extinction.
Methods
Mice
All experiments were performed using 8–16-week-old C57BL6/N (Samtako.Bio. Korea), ArcCreERT2 (ArcTRAP; Jackson Labs; stock #021881) and LSL-Cas9 (Jackson labs; stock #026175). Mice were raised in a 12-h light/dark cycle in standard laboratory cages and given ad libitum access to food and water. All procedures and animal care followed the regulations and guidelines of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees (IACUC) of Seoul National University or IBS (Daejeon, Korea).
DNA constructs
pAAV-FLEX-SaCas9-U6-sgRNA (Addgene plasmid # 124844; http://n2t.net/addgene:124844; RRID: Addgene_124844) and pAAV-FLEX-SaCas9-U6-sgGrin1(Addgene plasmid # 124852; http://n2t.net/addgene:124852; RRID:Addgene_124852) was a gift from Larry Zweifel. For the control sgRNA-containing cassette, the annealed oligos were inserted into the FLEX-SaCas9-U6-sgRNA plasmid following the author’s instruction [16].
Adeno-associated virus production
Briefly, plasmid containing the construct of interest flanked by AAV2 ITR, pAd-ΔF6, pRep/Cap9 were co-transfected into AAVpro® 293 T Cell Line (TAKARA, cat# 632273) and incubated in DMEM 10% v/v FBS for 5 days in 150 mm culture dish. On the harvest day, viruses in the culture medium were collected and remaining cells were broken to extract virus within it. After the centrifugation at 13,490×g for 20 min at 4 °C, supernatants were carefully loaded to the ultracentrifugation tubes filled with iodixanol gradient solutions as previously described (Fripont, 2019, Jove). 40% iodixanol solutions were aspirated using syringes and filtered by pre-rinsed centrifugal filters (Merck, cat# UFC910024).
Auditory fear conditioning
All mice were fear-conditioned 2–3 weeks after the AAV injection. Each mouse was habituated to the hands of the investigator and anesthesia chamber without isoflurane for 7 consecutive days. In all experiments, fear conditioning and extinction occurred in two different contexts (context A and context B) to minimize the influence of contextual associations. Context A consists of a square chamber with a steel grid floor (Coulbourn instruments; H10 - 11 M-TC), and context B consists of a rectangular plastic box with striped walls and with a hardwood laboratory bedding (betachip). 2 h before the conditioning, 250 μl of 5 mg/ml Doxycycline solution dissolved in saline was injected intraperitoneally during brief anesthesia by isoflurane. For auditory fear conditioning, mice were placed in context A and allowed to explore the context for 150 s, followed by three exposures to auditory tone CS (30 s), each of which co-terminated with 2 s, 0.75 mA footshock US, with a 30 s inter-trial interval (Lim et al., 2017). After the conditioning, mice were immediately delivered to their home cages. 1 day after the conditioning, mice were placed into a novel context B and exposed to the auditory tone to measure the freezing behavior. The freezing behavior was recorded and scored using a FreezeFrame fear-conditioning system.
Fear extinction
Four weeks after the auditory fear conditioning, all groups of mice underwent extinction. For two consecutive days, mice in the extinction group were placed into context B. After a 2-min exploration period, the auditory tone was administered 20 times with a 30-s inter-trial interval in the absence of the footshock. One day after the last extinction session, mice were placed into context B and exposed to the auditory CS to measure the freezing behavior. The extinction index was calculated by (Freezing of 1 st tone block of Extinction day 1 − Freezing of Retrieval 2)/(Freezing of 1 st tone block of Extinction day 1). Only animals with confirmed virus expression in both the BLA and the mPFC were included in the analysis.
Stereotaxic surgery
Mice were anesthetized with a ketamine/xylazine solution and positioned on a stereotaxic apparatus (Stoelting Co. Cat. No. 51733 or RWD). The virus mixture was injected into target regions using a 32-gauge needle with a Hamilton syringe at a rate of 0.125 μl/min. The total injection volume per site was 0.5 μl, and the tip of the needle was positioned 0.05 mm below the target coordinate right before the injection for 2 min. After the injection was completed, the needle stayed in place for an extra 6 min and was withdrawn slowly. Stereotaxic coordinates for each target site were: basolateral amygdala (AP: − 1.4/ML: ± 3.4/DV: − 5.05), medial prefrontal cortex (AP: + 1.8/ML: ± 0.4/DV: − 2.75), hippocampal CA1 (AP: + 1.75/ML: ± 1.4/DV: − 1.65 from dura).
Sample preparation and confocal imaging
Perfused brains were fixed with 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffered saline (PBS) overnight at 4 °C, and dehydrated in 30% sucrose in PBS for 2 days at 4 °C. Brains were sliced by Cryostat into a 40 μm section for immunohistochemistry. The BLA and the mPFC were imaged in a Z stack using a Leica SP8 confocal microscope with a 20× objective lens.
Electrophysiology
Mice were deeply anesthetized with ketamine and checked for tail-pinching reflex. Animals were then transcardially perfused with ice-cold sucrose-ACSF that contained (in mM): 210 sucrose, 3 KCl, 26 NaHCO3, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 5 MgSO4, 10 D-glucose, 3 sodium ascorbate and 0.5 CaCl2, saturated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2 (pH adjusted to 7.35 with HCl, 300–310 mOsm). Subsequently, the brain was removed and allowed to chill in the solution ~ 30 s. After mounting the brain on the agar block (3% w/v), slices were obtained using a vibratome (VT1200S, Leica) [17]. Transverse hippocampal slices or coronal slices containing BLA or mPFC were prepared and immediately transferred to normal-ACSF at 32–34 °C and allowed to recover there for 30 min. Normal ACSF contained (in mM): 124 NaCl, 3 KCl, 26 NaHCO3, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 2 MgSO4, 15 D-glucose and 1 or 2 CaCl2 (carbonated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2). Subsequently, slices were moved and allowed to recover at room temperature at least for 1 h before the recordings were made.
NMDA/AMPA ratio was measured using whole-cell solution that contained (in mM): 8 NaCl, 130 CsMeSO3, 10 HEPES, 0.5 EGTA, 4 Mg-ATP, 0.3 Na3-GTP, 5 QX- 314 and 0.1 spermine. Whole-cell recording was performed at 32 °C during continuous perfusion at 3–4 ml/min with ACSF that contained 100 μM picrotoxin. After 5 min of baseline recording, consecutive 10 responses to evoked EPSCs were measured in − 70 mV holding potential for AMPAR-currents. NMDAR-currents were estimated at 50 ms after the stimulation onset at + 40 mV of holding potential.
Immunohistochemistry
45 μm sections were rinsed three times in 1 × PBS. Sections were blocked for 1 h at room temperature in 1 × PBS with normal goat serum. Sections were incubated in primary antibody (rabbit anti-c-fos, Synaptic systems, 226,003 or 226,008; 1:1,000 in blocking solution; rabbit anti-HA, Sigma-Aldrich, H6908, 1:2000 in blocking solution; Mouse anti-GluN1, Synaptic Systems, 114 011; 1:500 in blocking solution) at 4°C for 16 h. After incubation, sections were rinsed three times for 5 min in 1 × PBS. Sections were incubated in secondary antibody (ThermoFisher, goat anti-rabbit 488, 1:500; Thermofisher, goat anti-mouse 647, 1:500) for 2 h at room temperature followed by a three-time rinse with 1 × PBS, with a second rinse for DAPI staining. Sections were mounted in VECTASHIELD mounting medium (Vector Laboratories). Processing of confocal images was performed using Imaris (Bitplane, Zurich, Switzerland) software.
Quantification and statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using Prism 10 (GraphPad). Datasets that passed the normality test were compared through a two-tailed unpaired t-test. Comparison of non-normal datasets was tested through a two-tailed Mann–Whitney test. For one-sample comparisons in Fig. 1H and I, Supplementary Fig. 2B, One-sample Wilcoxon test was used. For Fig. 1E and Supplementary Fig. 2D, two-way ANOVA was used to measure the group effect. The statistical test used, exact value of the sample size, and statistical significance are reported in each figure legend.
Data availability
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Abbreviations
- AFC :
-
Auditory fear conditioning
- CS :
-
Conditioned stimulus
- US :
-
Unconditioned stimulus
- NMDA :
-
N-methyl-d-aspartate
- AMPA :
-
α-Amino- 3-hydroxy- 5-methyl- 4-isoxazolepropionic acid
- mPFC :
-
Medial prefrontal cortex
- IL :
-
Infralimbic cortex
- BLA :
-
Basolateral amygdala
- AAV :
-
Adeno-associated virus
- DAPI :
-
4,6 Diamidino- 2-phenylindole
- KD :
-
Knockdown
- CRISPR :
-
Clustered regularly interspaced short palindromic repeats
- SaCas9 :
-
Cas9 from Staphylococcus aureus
- SaCas9 :
-
Cas9 from Staphylococcus pyogenes
- sgRNA :
-
Single guide RNA
- 4-OHT :
-
4-Hydroxytamoxifen
- IHC :
-
Immunohistochemistry
- EPSC :
-
Excitatory postsynaptic current
- EPSP :
-
Excitatory postsynaptic potential
References
Knapska E, Maren S. Reciprocal patterns of c-Fos expression in the medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala after extinction and renewal of conditioned fear. Learn Mem. 2009;16(8):486–93.
Do-Monte FH, Manzano-Nieves G, Quinones-Laracuente K, Ramos-Medina L, Quirk GJ. Revisiting the role of infralimbic cortex in fear extinction with optogenetics. J Neurosci. 2015;35(8):3607–15.
Laurent V, Westbrook RF. Distinct contributions of the basolateral amygdala and the medial prefrontal cortex to learning and relearning extinction of context conditioned fear. Learn Mem. 2008;15(9):657–66.
Senn V, Wolff SB, Herry C, Grenier F, Ehrlich I, Grundemann J, et al. Long-range connectivity defines behavioral specificity of amygdala neurons. Neuron. 2014;81(2):428–37.
Khalaf O, Resch S, Dixsaut L, Gorden V, Glauser L, Graff J. Reactivation of recall-induced neurons contributes to remote fear memory attenuation. Science. 2018;360(6394):1239–42.
Myers KM, Carlezon WA Jr, Davis M. Glutamate receptors in extinction and extinction-based therapies for psychiatric illness. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(1):274–93.
Zimmerman JM, Maren S. NMDA receptor antagonism in the basolateral but not central amygdala blocks the extinction of Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2010;31(9):1664–70.
Lee JLC, Nader K, Schiller D. An update on memory reconsolidation updating. Trends Cogn Sci. 2017;21(7):531–45.
Cui Z, Wang H, Tan Y, Zaia KA, Zhang S, Tsien JZ. Inducible and reversible NR1 knockout reveals crucial role of the NMDA receptor in preserving remote memories in the brain. Neuron. 2004;41(5):781–93.
Burgos-Robles A, Vidal-Gonzalez I, Santini E, Quirk GJ. Consolidation of fear extinction requires NMDA receptor-dependent bursting in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2007;53(6):871–80.
Santini E, Quirk GJ, Porter JT. Fear conditioning and extinction differentially modify the intrinsic excitability of infralimbic neurons. J Neurosci. 2008;28(15):4028–36.
Soler-Cedeno O, Torres-Rodriguez O, Bernard F, Maldonado L, Hernandez A, Porter JT. Plasticity of NMDA receptors at ventral hippocampal synapses in the infralimbic cortex regulates cued fear. eNeuro. 2019;6(2)
Roy DS, Park YG, Kim ME, Zhang Y, Ogawa SK, DiNapoli N, et al. Brain-wide mapping reveals that engrams for a single memory are distributed across multiple brain regions. Nat Commun. 2022;13(1):1799.
Choi DI, Kim J, Lee H, Kim JI, Sung Y, Choi JE, et al. Synaptic correlates of associative fear memory in the lateral amygdala. Neuron. 2021;109(17):2717–26.
Lacagnina AF, Brockway ET, Crovetti CR, Shue F, McCarty MJ, Sattler KP, et al. Distinct hippocampal engrams control extinction and relapse of fear memory. Nat Neurosci. 2019;22(5):753–61.
Hunker AC, Zweifel LS. Protocol to design, clone, and validate sgRNAs for in vivo reverse genetic studies. STAR Protoc. 2020;1(2):100070.
Kim S, Kim T, Lee HR, Jang EH, Ryu HH, Kang M, et al. Impaired learning and memory in CD38 null mutant mice. Mol Brain. 2016;9:16.
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Funding
This research was supported by the Institute for Basic Science (IBS), the Center for Cognition and Sociality (IBS-R001-D3) and the National Honor Scientist Program (NRF- 2012R1 A3 A1050385) of Korea.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Y.S., D.H.H., P.P and B.-K.K. designed the study. Y.S., D.H.H., P.P., J.K. performed the experiments and analyzed the data. Y.S., D.H.H., and B.-K.K. wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
All procedures were conducted under the animal care standards of the Institutional Animal Use and Care Committee.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
13041_2025_1203_MOESM1_ESM.pdf
Additional file 1: Schematics and electrophysiological validation of NMDAR KD strategies. A Schematics for NMDAR KD strategy in the mPFC and the BLA using AAV-SaCas9. B Schematics for NMDAR KD strategy in the hippocampal CA1 using AAV-SaCas9. C Left, sgGrin1 group showed a significantly decreased level of NMDA/AMPA ratio compared to the control group. Each dot represents a cell; grey, control group injected with scrambled sgRNA; orange, test group injected with sgGrin1. Right, representative EPSC trace of each group. Unpaired t-test, ***p= 0.001. D Schematics for NMDAR KD strategy in the hippocampal CA1 using AAV-SaCas9 using transgenic LSL-Cas9-EGFP mice. E Same for B except that recording slices were obtained from LSL-Cas9-EGFP mice injected with AAV-CaMKII-Cre-U6-sgGrin1; black, uninfected control cells; red, injected cells identified with EGFP fluorescence. Right, representative EPSC trace of each group. Unpaired t-test, ****p< 0.0001.
13041_2025_1203_MOESM2_ESM.pdf
Additional file 2; Immunohistochemical analysis of expression level of GluN1 and Cas9 proteins. A Representative images of colocalization analysis of remote recall-tagged tdT+ ensemble and SaCas9+ ensemble. B Colocalization ratio of the tdT+ and the SaCas9+ ensembles. Each dot represents the average value of images from an individual mouse. Left, HA-labeled Cas9+ cells within the BLA and the mPFC. N= 8. Right, colocalization ratio normalized to the chance level, chance level = P(tdT+|DAPI) x P(Cas9+|DAPI). One-sample Wilcoxon test, mPFC, ##p= 0.0078; BLA, ##p= 0.0078. C Representative images of GluN1 immunohistochemistry. D Left, normalized intensity of fluorescence stained against GluN1 in the tdT+ and tdT- neurons. Two-way ANOVA followed by Šídák's multiple comparisons test. sgGrin1 ATG group, tdT- group, n= 135; tdT+ group, n= 101 from 3 mice; sgGrin1 group, tdT- group, n= 105; tdT+ group, n= 97 from 3 mice; Two-way ANOVA, tdT effect, ****p< 0.0001, Grin1 KD effect, ****p< 0.0001, interaction, ****p< 0.0001; Šídák's multiple comparisons test, sgGrin1 ATG group, ns, adjusted p> 0.9999, sgGrin1 group, ****p< 0.0001; Right, The GluN1 immunofluorescence of the GluN1+ tdT- cells. Mann-Whitney test, ns, p= 0.2210.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Sung, Y., Han, D.H., Kim, J. et al. Targeted NMDA receptor knockdown in recall‐activated neuronal ensembles impairs remote fear extinction. Mol Brain 18, 30 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13041-025-01203-z
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13041-025-01203-z