Skip to main content

Post-conditioning sleep deprivation facilitates delay and trace fear memory extinction

Abstract

Trace and delay auditory fear conditioning involve different memory association strategies based on working memory involvement; however, their differences in long-term processing through sleep and extinction training remain unclear. While females often exhibit more persistent fear, complicating psychiatric treatment, most studies have primarily focused on how sleep affects initial recall in male mice. We investigated the three-way interaction between tests (trace vs. delay), sleep states, and sex during initial recall, extinction, and post-extinction remote recall. A six-hour post-conditioning sleep deprivation (SD) did not affect freezing behavior during the following day’s extinction training of delay fear memory. However, during post-extinction remote recall of delay fear memory, SD prevented spontaneous recovery in males and reduced persistent freezing in females. In contrast, SD rapidly facilitated extinction of trace fear memory. In summary, SD enhances extinction both in the short-term and long-term, depending on the conditioning protocol. These findings highlight the importance of long-term assessments to explore interactions among emotional memory, sleep, and sex differences, with implications for individualized mechanisms underlying post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and its treatments.

Introduction

Newly acquired memories are consolidated during subsequent sleep for long-term storage [1, 2]. The role of sleep in memory consolidation has been explored through various behavioral tests in rodents. Studies show that sleep deprivation (SD) impairs memory consolidation in hippocampus-dependent tests, such as contextual fear conditioning and object-place recognition, but not in tests less reliant on the hippocampus, such as auditory delay fear conditioning and the novel object recognition test [3,4,5]. Recent studies, however, show that while newly formed memories in novel object recognition do not depend on hippocampal activity during sleep, remote memories do [6, 7]. Additionally, memory consolidation in auditory delay fear conditioning is influenced by the inactivation of prefrontal interneurons [8] but not by hippocampal theta waves during rapid-eye movement (REM) sleep [9]. These findings underscore the importance of considering multiple brain regions and time points to understand the relationships between sleep and memory.

Fear conditioning and extinction training are well-established rodent models for studying post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and exposure therapy. Extinction training decreases learned fear through repeated, nonreinforced presentations of the conditioned stimulus (CS) without the aversive unconditioned stimulus (US). This process does not completely erase the original CS–US association but creates a safety memory that competes with the fear memory [10,11,12]. Persistent fear memories can overshadow safety memories, leading to spontaneous recovery of fear responses over time and complicating treatment efforts [13, 14]. PTSD prevalence is twice as high in females as in males [15], and numerous studies have demonstrated slower extinction in females, both in humans and animals [16,17,18,19,20]. Therefore, we aimed to reveal the long-term dynamics of fear memory and extinction in both sexes to enhance our understanding and treatment of psychiatric disorders.

Among behavioral tests in rodents, auditory cued fear conditioning is particularly advantageous for studying associative memory due to its precise temporal control between CS and US. The direct coupling of the CS and US in delay fear conditioning produces stronger freezing behavior [21]. However, natural fearful situations can include an interval between cue stimuli and imminent danger (e.g., rustling leaves prior to a predator’s approach). Similarly, in the phenomenology of PTSD, intrusive memories include not only stimuli present during the traumatic event but also those encountered shortly beforehand [22]. Trace fear conditioning, which requires working memory to link CS and US through an interval, involves more engagement of the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex [23,24,25,26]. Although the delay and trace fear conditioning undergo different processes, their long-term processing through sleep and extinction training remains unclear. We hypothesized that trace fear memory is more susceptible to sleep, as global inter-regional synchronization serves for internal information integration [27]. Here, we assessed interactions among test (delay vs. trace), state (sleep vs. SD), and sex in freezing behavior during initial recall, extinction, and post-extinction remote recall.

Materials and methods

Animals

Male and female C57BL/6J mice, aged 7 weeks, were purchased from SLC (Shizuoka, Japan). The mice were between 2 and 3 months old at the start of behavioral tests. Prior to the behavioral tests, they were singly housed for at least one week and handled by a male experimenter for three consecutive days. The animals were kept on a 12-hour light-dark cycle with food and water provided ad libitum. Home cages (155 × 245 × 148 mm, lwh) were stored in four ventilated soundproof boxes (60 × 70 × 60 cm, lwh) with staggered light-on times (9:30, 10:00, 10:30, or 11:00) to allow four consecutive behavioral experiments with four animals at the same circadian timing. Each behavioral session ended at Zeitgeber time (ZT0) in order to initiate post-conditioning 6-hour SD at the onset of the inactive period. SD was performed by gently touching the mice with a cotton swab during immobile timings, as visually detected by male and female experimenters.

Delay fear conditioning

A black polyvinyl chloride behavioral chamber (166 × 146 × 238 mm, lwh) was placed inside a soundproof box (60 × 70 × 60 cm, lwh). The behavioral chamber was wiped with 70% ethanol after each behavioral session for each mouse. The same chamber was used for both delay and trace fear conditioning. Day 0 (Habituation): After a 2-minute baseline period in the chamber (20 lux lighting with cone bedding), 4 CS- (2 kHz) and 4 CS+ (10 kHz) tone (30 s, 75 dB) were presented with a pseudo-random inter-trial interval (45 to 90 s) in an intermingled manner. Day 1 (Conditioning): Following a 2-minute baseline period with a metal grid floor, 5 CS- and 5 CS+ were applied with a pseudo-random inter-trial interval of 45 to 90 s in an intermingled manner. At the end of CS+, a foot shock (0.5 s, 0.4 A) was delivered by a shock generator (Ohara & Co. Ltd., Tokyo, Japan). Day 2 (Extinction #1): After a 3-minute baseline period in the Habituation chamber, 4 CS- and then 12 CS+ were applied with a pseudo-random inter-trial interval of 45 to 90 s. Day 17 (Remote recall): After a 2-minute baseline period in the Habituation chamber, 4 CS- and the subsequent 4 CS+ were applied with a pseudo-random inter-trial interval of 45 to 90 s. Day 18 (Extinction #2): The same configuration as on Day 2.

Trace fear conditioning

Day 0 (Habituation): Following a 2-minute baseline, 4 CS+ tone (10 kHz, 10 s, 75 dB) were presented with a 230-second inter-tone interval. Day 1 (Conditioning): After a 2-minute baseline, 5 CS+ followed by a 20-second trace period and a foot shock (0.5 s, 0.4 A) were presented with a 230-second inter-tone interval. Day 2 (Extinction #1): After a 3-minute baseline, 16 CS+ were presented with a pseudo-random inter-tone interval (65 to 110 s). Day 17 (Remote recall): The same configuration as on Day 0. Day 18 (Extinction #2): The same configuration as on Day 2.

Data acquisition and analysis

The foot shock generator and tone function generator (OPR-SS2T, Ohara & Co. Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) were triggered by the OTPG-8 and the Fiberphotometry & Electrophysiology Console (Doric Lenses, Quebec, Canada). Animal motion was recorded via a webcam, and freezing scores were calculated using the Behavior Analysis module in Doric Neuroscience Studio. MATLAB was used for statistical analyses and freezing score conversion, with percentages calculated during the tone period for delay fear conditioning and both the cue and trace periods for trace fear conditioning.

A three-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) assessed freezing during the last three CS+ and three CS- trials on Day 1 for delay fear conditioning, the last three CS+ trials on Day 1 for trace fear conditioning, and the first four trials on Day 2 for both tests. The Day 1 trials were used for the covariate in analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). Additionally, a two-way ANOVA evaluated freezing during the last three CS+ trials on Day 1, the early and late six CS+ trials on Day 2 and Day 18, or all four trials on Day 17. Those trials from Day 2, Day 17, and Day 18 served as dependent variables in the ANCOVA.

Results

Auditory fear conditioning protocols and behavioral schedule

Auditory stimuli were paired with electrical shocks in both delay and trace fear conditioning (Fig. 1A). In delay fear conditioning (Fig. 1A1), the paired conditioned stimulus (CS+) was followed by an electrical shock at its termination, while the unpaired conditioned stimulus (CS-) was not. In trace fear conditioning (Fig. 1A2), the CS+ was followed by a shock after a 20-second trace interval. Mice underwent either delay or trace fear conditioning on Day 1, followed by re-exposure to the auditory stimuli during Extinction #1 on Day 2, Remote recall on Day 17, and Extinction #2 on Day 18 (Fig. 1B). After conditioning, mice were either allowed to sleep (S) or subjected to 6-hour SD.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Protocols for delay and trace fear conditioning, followed by recall and extinction sessions. (A) In delay fear conditioning, an electrical foot shock was paired with the end of each CS+ (30 s, 10 kHz), while CS- (30 s, 2 kHz) was presented without a foot shock. In trace fear conditioning, the foot shock was delivered 20 s after the CS+ (30 s, 10 kHz). (B) Following auditory fear conditioning on Day 1, male and female mice were re-exposed to tone stimuli without the foot shock for Extinction #1 on Day 2, the Remote recall on Day 17, and Extinction #2 on Day 18. A subset of mice underwent 6-hour SD after auditory fear conditioning

Higher initial memory retrieval in delay fear conditioning compared to trace fear conditioning, and in females compared to males

We examined whether sex (male vs. female), test (delay vs. trace fear conditioning), or state (S vs. SD) influenced memory acquisition and initial recall. A three-way ANOVA (Table 1) on the last trials of Day 1 found no significant main effects for sex, test, or state, nor any interactions between these factors (Fig. 2A). However, during initial trials on Day 2, significant main effects were found for sex (F (1, 70) = 12.27, p = 0.0008) and test (F (1, 70) = 32.44, p < 0.0001), with no main effects of state or interactions (Fig. 2B). This indicates that females and mice in the delay conditioning exhibited higher freezing during initial fear memory recall, which was not affected by post-conditioning SD. We then examined the time course of freezing during recall and extinction sessions for each test and sex.

Table 1 Three-way ANOVA results for the last trials on day 1 and the initial trials on day 2
Fig. 2
figure 2

Higher freezing in initial recall in delay fear conditioning and in females. (A) During the last trials on Day 1, no significant differences were observed across test, sex, or state. (B) During the initial trials on Day 2, higher freezing levels were observed in the delay fear conditioning group and in females (**p < 0.01 for the main effect of the test factor, ##p < 0.01 for the main effect of the sex factor, three-way ANOVA, Table 1). Sample sizes for the delay fear conditioning group were n = 10 for male S, n = 10 for male SD, n = 9 for female S, and n = 10 for female SD mice. In the trace fear conditioning group, sample sizes were n = 10 for male S, n = 10 for male SD, n = 10 for female S, and n = 9 for female SD mice. Values represent the mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM)

Post-conditioning SD enhanced gradual fear extinction of delay fear memory

In males, both the S (n = 10) and SD (n = 10) groups exhibited increased freezing in response to both CS+ (Fig. 3A1) and CS- (Fig. 3A2) during delay fear conditioning on Day 1. During Extinction #1 on Day 2, both groups showed a significant decrease in freezing; however, only the S group restored high freezing levels during the Remote recall on Day 17, comparable to their initial recall on Day 2 (Fig. 3A1). Both male S and SD groups further reduced freezing to CS+ during Extinction #2 on Day 18 (Fig. 3A1). Additionally, low-level freezing was observed upon re-exposure to CS- on Day 2, 17, and 18 (Fig. 3A2), suggesting that SD did not affect motor functions or general fear or anxiety in male mice.

In females, both the S (n = 9) and SD (n = 10) groups showed increased freezing to CS+ (Fig. 3B1) and CS- (Fig. 3B2) during delay fear conditioning on Day 1. During Extinction #1 on Day 2, neither group showed significant decreases in freezing, with only mild reductions noted in the SD group (Fig. 3B1). In contrast, the SD group, but not the S group, exhibited significant reductions in freezing during post-extinction remote recall on Day 17 and in the early trials of Day 18. Unlike male S group (Fig. 3A1), the female S and SD groups did not show increased freezing from Day 2 to 17, potentially due to less effective extinction training with persistently high freezing on Day 2 (Fig. 3B1). As in males, low freezing responses to CS- were observed on Day 2, 17, and 18 in both female S and SD groups (Fig. 3B2).

A two-way ANOVA (Table 2) showed significant main effects of sex across early (Fig. 3C2, F (1, 35) = 11.61, p = 0.0017) and late (Fig. 3C3, F (1, 35) = 18.84, p = 0.0001) trials on Day 2, Day 17 (Fig. 3C4, F (1, 35) = 9.37, p = 0.0042), early (Fig. 3C5, F (1, 35) = 11.93, p = 0.0015) and late (Fig. 3C6, F (1, 35) = 6.36, p = 0.0164) trials on Day 18. Significant main effects of state were observed on Day 17 (Fig. 3C4, F (1, 35) = 8.56, p = 0.006) and early Day 18 (Fig. 3C5, F (1, 35) = 7.4, p = 0.0101). These results indicate that while sex influenced freezing during each set of trials for delay fear memory recall, post-conditioning SD reduced freezing during post-extinction remote recall in both males and females.

Table 2 Two-way ANOVA results for delay and trace fear conditioning
Fig. 3
figure 3

In delay fear conditioning, post-conditioning SD enhanced gradual fear extinction. (A1) Male S mice (n = 10) showed decreased freezing during CS+ from Day 2, followed by Remote recall on Day 17 and re-extinction on Day 18. Male SD mice (n = 10) decreased freezing during CS+ on Day 2 without spontaneous recovery on Day 17 (*p < 0.05 and **p < 0.01 for S mice, #p < 0.05 and ##p < 0.01 for SD mice, multiple paired t-tests vs. initial 4 trials on Day 2). (A2) CS- triggered low-level freezing in male S and SD mice. (B1) Female S mice (n = 9) demonstrated decreased freezing during CS+ from Day 17. Female SD mice (n = 10) decreased freezing during CS+ from late trials on Day 2 without spontaneous recovery on Day 17. (*p < 0.05 and **p < 0.01 for S mice, #p < 0.05 and ##p < 0.01 for SD mice, multiple paired t-tests versus initial 4 trials on Day 2). (B2) CS- triggered low-level freezing in female S and SD mice. (C1) Similar freezing during CS+ was observed across sexes and states on Day 1. (C2 − 6) Female mice showed higher freezing during CS+ across early (C2) and late (C3) Day 2, Day 17 (C4), and early (C5) and late (C6) Day 18. SD reduced freezing during CS+ on Day 17 (C4) and early Day 18 (C5). (*p < 0.05 and **p < 0.01 for the main effect of the state factor, #p < 0.05 and ##p < 0.01 for the main effect of the sex factor, two-way ANOVA, Table 2). Values represent the mean ± SEM

Fig. 4
figure 4

In trace fear conditioning, post-conditioning SD accelerated extinction. (A1) Male S mice (n = 10) showed mild decreases in freezing on Day 2 and strong decreases on Day 18. Male SD mice (n = 10) exhibited decreased freezing on Day 2 with further decreases on Day 18 (*p < 0.05 and **p < 0.01 for S mice, #p < 0.05 and ##p < 0.01 for SD mice, multiple paired t-tests vs. initial 4 trials on Day 2). (A2) Female S mice (n = 10) decreased freezing from Day 2 with further decreases on Day 18. Female SD mice (n = 9) showed strong decreases in freezing on Day 2 to levels comparable to Day 18. (*p < 0.05 and **p < 0.01 for S mice, #p < 0.05 and ##p < 0.01 for SD mice, multiple paired t-tests vs. initial 4 trials on Day 2). (B) SD decreased freezing on later Day 2 (B3) with no significant sex differences from Day 1 (B1), early (B2) and late (B3) Day 2, Day 17 (B4), to early (B5) and late (B6) Day18. (*p < 0.05 for state factor, two-way ANOVA, Table 2). Values represent the mean ± SEM

Post-conditioning SD accelerated trace fear extinction

Male S mice (n = 10) showed mild reductions in freezing on Day 2 and 17, with a more substantial decrease on Day 18 (Fig. 4A1). Male SD mice (n = 10), however, exhibited significant reductions on Day 2 and 17, with further reductions on Day 18. In females, while both S (n = 10) and SD (n = 9) mice began to decrease freezing on Day 2, with the SD group showing a more pronounced reduction (Fig. 4A2). A two-way ANOVA (Table 2) revealed significant main effects of state at late Day 2 (Fig. 4B3, F (1, 35) = 4.86, p = 0.0341), but no significant main effects of sex or interactions between state and sex. Thus, post-conditioning SD accelerated extinction of trace fear memory (Fig. 4B3) at an earlier stage than delay fear memory (Fig. 3C4, 5).

Controlling for inter-individual variability: ANCOVA results

Freezing during fear conditioning and recall is influenced by variability arising from individual differences in responsiveness to neutral and aversive stimuli, associative learning ability, defense strategies such as freeze or flight, among other factors. In females, this variability can also be affected by the estrous cycle. To account for these variations, we performed an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), using freezing on Day 1 as a covariate, to isolate the effects of SD on recall and extinction while controlling for differences in memory acquisition.

In delay fear conditioning, ANCOVA (Table 3) revealed that SD significantly reduced freezing on Day 17 in male mice (Fig. 5A3, F (1, 17) = 8.12, p = 0.022), and on Day 17 (Fig. 5B3, F (1, 16) = 31.54, p = 0.004) and early Day 18 (Fig. 5B4, F (1, 16) = 18.71, p = 0.002) in females. In trace fear conditioning, SD significantly decreased freezing in females on late Day 2 (Fig. 6B2, F (1, 16) = 5.70, p = 0.021). The positive slope of all linear regression lines (Figs. 5 and 6) indicates that inter-individual variability was maintained across sessions. Thus, the enhancement of extinction in both delay and trace fear memory by post-conditioning SD is not due to differences in individual variability during memory acquisition.

Table 3 ANCOVA results for freezing behavior in recall sessions
Fig. 5
figure 5

Post-conditioning SD gradually enhanced delay fear extinction, independent of pre-existing group differences. (A, B) Scatter plots and linear regression lines representing freezing behavior in male (A) and female (B) mice during each set of CS+ recall trials compared to conditioning on Day 1. Group differences between S and SD were tested while accounting for freezing on Day 1 as a covariate (ANCOVA, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, Table 3). Each set of re-exposure corresponds to early Day 2 (A1, B1), late Day 2 (A2, B2), Day 17 (A3, B3), early Day 18 (A4, B4), and late Day 18 (A5, B5). SD decreased freezing during Remote recall in male mice (A3) and during Remote recall (B3) and early Extinction #2 (B4) in female mice

Fig. 6
figure 6

Post-conditioning SD accelerated trace fear extinction, independent of pre-existing group differences. (A, B) Scatter plots and linear regression lines representing freezing behavior in male (A) and female (B) mice during each set of CS+ recall trials compared to conditioning on Day 1. Group differences between S and SD were tested while accounting for freezing on Day 1 as a covariate (ANCOVA, *p < 0.05, Table 3). Each set of recall trials corresponds to early Day 2 (A1, B1), late Day 2 (A2, B2), Day 17 (A3, B3), early Day 18 (A4, B4), and late Day 18 (A5, B5). SD decreased freezing during late Extinction #21 in female mice (B2)

Discussion

Post-conditioning SD enhances early trace fear extinction and gradual delay fear extinction

Across both sexes and test protocols in auditory fear conditioning, we consistently found that post-conditioning SD preserved initial recall (Fig. 2B) but reduced freezing during later recall. This consistent reduction may be attributed to our focus on post-conditioning periods for fear memory consolidation, as post-extinction periods likely involve combined processes such as fear memory reconsolidation and extinction memory consolidation. SD effects emerged during trace fear extinction (Fig. 4B3) and after delay fear extinction (Fig. 3C4, 3C5), indicating that SD makes fear memory more susceptible to extinction in a test- and timing-dependent manner (Table 4). While the rapid facilitation of Extinction #1 by SD in trace fear conditioning is not attributed to impairment of reconsolidation, the reduction in freezing by SD after Extinction #1 in delay fear conditioning may reflect impairment of reconsolidation circuits, facilitation of extinction circuits, or both. From these findings and speculation, it seems that SD influences multi-regional processing necessary for recent trace and remote delay fear memory.

Different brain mechanisms underlie the distinctions between delay and trace fear memory in both conditioning and extinction. Trace fear memory requires working memory and awareness [28], necessitating greater involvement of the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus during conditioning [23]. During extinction, while the infralimbic cortex is activated in both tests, the amygdala is necessary for delay fear memory extinction, whereas the retrosplenial cortex is required for trace fear memory extinction [29]. Furthermore, emotional memory circuits, including the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, thalamus, and amygdala, communicate inter-regionally while establishing fear and extinction memory engrams [30]. In remote memory, neural coding can drift within each brain region [31] and shift across brain regions through systems consolidation [32, 33]. Besides neural activity, the remote effects of SD in extinction recall may also be attributed to sleep-dependent long-term inter-regional synaptic plasticity and relevant molecules [34]. To address dynamic and heterogeneous neural populations and their interactions across long-time course, we have constructed fiber photometry systems targeting multiple brain regions with cell-type specificity and sleep-state resolution [35,36,37].

In addition to the possible qualitative differences in brain mechanisms, a quantitative difference also underlies the distinction between delay and trace fear memory. Although mice show higher freezing during initial recall of delay fear memory (Fig. 2B), the test-dependent SD effects are unlikely to be due to quantitative differences, as the freezing levels during initial recall are similar between males in delay fear memory and females in trace fear memory. Additionally, SD consistently decreased freezing during post-extinction remote recall in males and females, who exhibited different freezing levels across the sets of recall trials (Fig. 3C2 − 6). While we consistently used 5 CS+–US association trials across tests and sexes, adjusting this number (e.g. 4 trials for delay vs. 6 trials for trace) may help align freezing levels [38].

Table 4 Timeline summary of freezing decrease by SD in delay and trace fear conditioning

Sex differences in recall and extinction

In delay fear extinction, male mice decreased freezing during Extinction #1 and recovered freezing in Remote recall (Fig. 3A1), while females showed persistently high levels of freezing (Fig. 3B1). These sex differences align with previous reports and have been linked to neural activity in various brain regions, including the prefrontal cortex, insular cortex, hippocampus, amygdala, and hypothalamus, all of which have receptors for sex hormones [39]. Among sex hormones, estradiol administration facilitates extinction in ovariectomized female rats [40]. Also, female rats in the proestrus phase during extinction (Day 2) have been reported to exhibit lower freezing across conditioning (Day 1), extinction (Day 2), and test (Day 3) compared to those in the metestrus phase [21]. Many studies have used delay fear conditioning for testing sex differences or estrous cycle dependency, some studies showed higher freezing in females in trace fear memory [41], which is consistent to our study (Fig. 2B), and acquisition of trace fear conditioning is less reliant on muscarinic signals in prelimbic cortex in females [42]. Thus, both delay and trace fear memory have sex differences in the brain processing mechanisms.

We observed no significant differences between S and SD groups of females during conditioning for both delay and trace conditioning (Fig. 2A), suggesting that an unintended significant bias due to the estrous cycle at the start of experiments is unlikely in these two groups. This was further supported by ANCOVA controlling the effect of memory acquisition as a covariate. The significant decrease of fear memory recall in the SD group occurred without significant changes in the linear regression slope (Figs. 5 and 6), suggesting that SD affects emotional memory processing while maintaining inter-individual variability in freezing levels. Because the estrous cycle can affect sleep states [43], interaction among emotional memory circuits, sleep state, and estrous cycle should be an important question in this field. A recent study in rats shows that vertex auditory responses are enhanced at the proestrus stage across sleep/wake states, particularly during REM sleep [44]. Further study is needed to understand how the estrous cycle and sleep interact in the processing of associative auditory fear memory.

Limitations

A limitation of this study is that SD affects global brain states, making it difficult to identify specific brain regions responsible for extinction facilitation. Although SD impacts the entire brain, it did not alter general fear or anxiety levels, as evidenced by the lack of significant changes in freezing to CS- (Fig. 3A2; Fig. 3B2). SD with gentle stimulation is known not to elevate corticosterone levels [45], which can impair memory consolidation [46]. After a 6-hour SD period, rebound sleep tends to occur, especially within the first 2 hour, and natural EEG patterns resume within 8 hour [47]. To avoid overlap between rebound sleep and behavioral sessions, we implemented a 24-hour interval between conditioning on Day 1 and Extinction #1 on Day 2. Previous studies from our group and others suggest that behavioral testing at different circadian phases supports the role of post-learning sleep [48, 49]. Our findings align with these studies, showing lower freezing during post-extinction remote recall in the active phase (ZT16) compared to the inactive phase (ZT4) [50]. However, these approaches do not distinguish the roles of non-REM and REM sleep, even though fear conditioning fragments REM sleep [51] and increases non-REM sleep [52]. Although sleep-state specific optogenetic manipulations have been applied to delay fear memory [8, 9, 53], further application is required particularly for trace fear memory.

Conclusion

Using both male and female mice, we found that post-conditioning SD rapidly enhanced trace fear extinction and gradually enhanced delay fear extinction. The advantages of auditory fear conditioning include trial-averaged or time-locked analysis of neural activity during CS and US. Further studies investigating long-term interactions among memory types, sleep, and sex differences are needed to improve our understanding of individualized emotional memory storage and resilience.

Data availability

The data supporting the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ANCOVA:

Analysis of Covariance

ANOVA:

Analysis of Variance

CS:

Conditioned Stimulus

PTSD:

Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder

REM:

Rapid-Eye Movement

SD:

Sleep Deprivation

SEM:

Standard Error of the Mean

US:

Unconditioned Stimulus

ZT:

Zeitgeber Time

References

  1. Brodt S, Inostroza M, Niethard N, Born J. Sleep-A brain-state serving systems memory consolidation. Neuron. 2023;111:1050–75. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/37023710/

  2. Miyamoto D. Neural circuit plasticity for complex non-declarative sensorimotor memory consolidation during sleep. Neurosci Res. 2023;189:37–43. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/36584925/

  3. Ishikawa H, Yamada K, Pavlides C, Ichitani Y. Sleep deprivation impairs spontaneous object-place but not novel-object recognition in rats. Neurosci Lett. 2014;580:114–8. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/25123440/

  4. Inostroza M, Binder S, Born J. Sleep-dependency of episodic-like memory consolidation in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2013;237:15–22. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22989412/

  5. Graves LA, Heller EA, Pack AI, Abel T. Sleep deprivation selectively impairs memory consolidation for contextual fear conditioning. Learn Mem. 2003;10:168–76. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12773581/

  6. Sawangjit A, Harkotte M, Oyanedel CN, Niethard N, Born J, Inostroza M. Two distinct ways to form long-term object recognition memory during sleep and wakefulness. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2022;119:e2203165119. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1073/pnas.2203165119

  7. Sawangjit A, Oyanedel CN, Niethard N, Salazar C, Born J, Inostroza M. The hippocampus is crucial for forming non-hippocampal long-term memory during sleep. Nature. 2018;564:109–13. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30429612/

  8. Aime M, Calcini N, Borsa M, Campelo T, Rusterholz T, Sattin A et al. Paradoxical somatodendritic decoupling supports cortical plasticity during REM sleep. Science. 2022;376:724–30. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1126/science.abk2734

  9. Boyce R, Glasgow SD, Williams S, Adamantidis A. Causal evidence for the role of REM sleep theta rhythm in contextual memory consolidation. Science. 2016;352:812–6. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/27174984/

  10. Nguyen R, Koukoutselos K, Forro T, Ciocchi S. Fear extinction relies on ventral hippocampal safety codes shaped by the amygdala. Sci Adv. 2023;9. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/37256958/

  11. Myers KM, Davis M. Mechanisms of fear extinction. Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12:120–50. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17160066/

  12. Bouton ME. Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learn Mem. 2004;11:485–94. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15466298/

  13. Schiller D, Cain CK, Curley NG, Schwartz JS, Stern SA, LeDoux JE et al. Evidence for recovery of fear following immediate extinction in rats and humans. Learn Mem. 2008;15:394–402. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18509113/

  14. Lacagnina AF, Brockway ET, Crovetti CR, Shue F, McCarty MJ, Sattler KP et al. Distinct hippocampal engrams control extinction and relapse of fear memory. Nat Neurosci. 2019;22:753–61. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30936555/

  15. Lebron-Milad K, Milad MR. Sex differences, gonadal hormones and the fear extinction network: implications for anxiety disorders. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord. 2012;2. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22738383/

  16. Fenton GE, Pollard AK, Halliday DM, Mason R, Bredy TW, Stevenson CW. Persistent prelimbic cortex activity contributes to enhanced learned fear expression in females. Learn Mem. 2014;21:55–60. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24429423/

  17. Baker-Andresen D, Flavell CR, Li X, Bredy TW. Activation of BDNF signaling prevents the return of fear in female mice. Learn Mem. 2013;20:237–40. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/23589089/

  18. Milad MR, Orr SP, Klibanski A, Goldstein JM, Wedig MM, Pitman RK et al. Fear conditioning and extinction: influence of sex and menstrual cycle in healthy humans. Behavioral neuroscience. 2006;120:1196–203. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17201462/

  19. Glover EM, Jovanovic T, Mercer KB, Kerley K, Bradley B, Ressler KJ et al. Estrogen levels are associated with extinction deficits in women with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;72:19–24. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22502987/

  20. Baran SE, Armstrong CE, Niren DC, Hanna JJ, Conrad CD. Chronic stress and sex differences on the recall of fear conditioning and extinction. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2009;91:323–32. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19073269/

  21. Milad MR, Igoe SA, Lebron-Milad K, Novales JE. Estrous cycle phase and gonadal hormones influence conditioned fear extinction. Neuroscience. 2009;164:887–95. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19761818/

  22. Ehlers A, Hackmann A, Steil R, Clohessy S, Wenninger K, Winter H. The nature of intrusive memories after trauma: the warning signal hypothesis. Behav Res Ther. 2002;40:995–1002. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12296496/

  23. Esclassan F, Coutureau E, Di Scala G, Marchand AR. Differential contribution of dorsal and ventral hippocampus to trace and delay fear conditioning. Hippocampus. 2009;19:33–44. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1002/hipo.20473

  24. Burman MA, Starr MJ, Gewirtz JC. Dissociable effects of hippocampus lesions on expression of fear and trace fear conditioning memories in rats. Hippocampus. 2006;16:103–13. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1002/hipo.20137

  25. Purple RJ, Sakurai T, Sakaguchi M. Auditory conditioned stimulus presentation during NREM sleep impairs fear memory in mice. Sci Rep. 2017;7. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28401950/

  26. Miyawaki H, Mizuseki K. De novo inter-regional coactivations of preconfigured local ensembles support memory. Nat Commun. 2022;13. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/35277492/

  27. Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron. 2014;81:12–34. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24411729/

  28. Weike AI, Schupp HT, Hamm AO. Fear acquisition requires awareness in trace but not delay conditioning. Psychophysiology. 2007;44:170–80. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17241153/

  29. Kwapis JL, Jarome TJ, Lee JL, Gilmartin MR, Helmstetter FJ. Extinguishing trace fear engages the retrosplenial cortex rather than the amygdala. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;113:41–54. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24055593/

  30. Luft JG, Popik B, Gonçalves DA, Cruz FC, de Oliveira Alvares L. Distinct engrams control fear and extinction memory. Hippocampus. 2024;34:230–40. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/38396226/

  31. Zaki Y, Cai DJ. Memory engram stability and flexibility. Neuropsychopharmacology 2024. 2024;1–9. https://www.nature.com/articles/s41386-024-01979-z

  32. Kitamura T, Ogawa SK, Roy DS, Okuyama T, Morrissey MD, Smith LM et al. Engrams and circuits crucial for systems consolidation of a memory. Science. 2017;356:73-8. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1126/science.aam6808

  33. Goto A, Bota A, Miya K, Wang J, Tsukamoto S, Jiang X et al. Stepwise synaptic plasticity events drive the early phase of memory consolidation. Science. 2021;374:857–63. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/34762472/

  34. Miyamoto D, Marshall W, Tononi G, Cirelli C. Net decrease in spine-surface GluA1-containing AMPA receptors after post-learning sleep in the adult mouse cortex. Nat Commun. 2021;12. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/34001888/

  35. Miyamoto D. Optical imaging and manipulation of sleeping-brain dynamics in memory processing. Neurosci Res. 2022;181:9–16. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/35439575/

  36. Miyamoto D. Multi-fiber photometry of multi-region information integration in the prefrontal cortex during fear conditioning and sleep. Toyama Med J, in press.

  37. Miyamoto D, Murayama M. The fiber-optic imaging and manipulation of neural activity during animal behavior. Neurosci Res. 2016;103:1–9. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26427958/

  38. Kwapis JL, Jarome TJ, Schiff JC, Helmstetter FJ. Memory consolidation in both trace and delay fear conditioning is disrupted by intra-amygdala infusion of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin. Learn Memory. 2011;18:728. https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC3207254/

  39. Velasco ER, Florido A, Milad MR, Andero R. Sex differences in fear extinction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;103:81–108. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31129235/

  40. Graham BM, Daher M. Estradiol and progesterone have opposing roles in the regulation of fear extinction in female rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:774–80. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26156559/

  41. Wiltgen BJ, Sanders MJ, Ferguson C, Homanics GE, Fanselow MS. Trace fear conditioning is enhanced in mice lacking the δ subunit of the GABAA receptor. Learning & Memory. 2005;12:327.

  42. Kirry AJ, Durigan DJ, Twining RC, Gilmartin MR. Estrous cycle stage gates sex differences in prefrontal muscarinic control of fear memory formation. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2019;161:26–36. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30851433/

  43. Swift KM, Keus K, Echeverria CG, Cabrera Y, Jimenez J, Holloway J et al. Sex differences within sleep in gonadally intact rats. Sleep. 2020;43. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31784755/

  44. Tóth A, Traub M, Bencsik N, Détári L, Hajnik T, Dobolyi A. Sleep- and sleep deprivation-related changes of vertex auditory evoked potentials during the estrus cycle in female rats. Sci Rep. 2024;14. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/38461157/

  45. Foilb AR, Taylor-Yeremeeva EM, Schmidt BD, Ressler KJ, William A, Carlezon Jr. Acute sleep deprivation reduces fear memories in male and female mice. bioRxiv. 2024.

  46. Brosens N, Lesuis SL, Bassie I, Reyes L, Gajadien P, Lucassen PJ et al. Elevated corticosterone after fear learning impairs remote auditory memory retrieval and alters brain network connectivity. Learn Memory. 2023;30:125.

  47. Huber R, Deboer TOM, Tobler I. Topography of EEG dynamics after sleep deprivation in mice. J Neurophysiol. 2000;84:1888–93. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11024081/

  48. Miyamoto D, Hirai D, Fung CCA, Inutsuka A, Odagawa M, Suzuki T et al. Top-down cortical input during NREM sleep consolidates perceptual memory. Science. 2016;352:1315–8. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/27229145/

  49. Inostroza M, Binder S, Born J. Sleep-dependency of episodic-like memory consolidation in rats. Behavioural brain research. 2013;237:15–22. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22989412/

  50. Hartsock MJ, Levy CT, Navarro MJ, Saddoris MP, Spencer RL. Circadian rhythms in conditioned threat extinction reflect time-of-day differences in ventromedial prefrontal cortex neural processing. J Neurosci. 2024;44:e0878242024. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/39251355/

  51. DaSilva JK, Lei Y, Madan V, Mann GL, Ross RJ, Tejani-Butt S, et al. Fear conditioning fragments REM sleep in stress-sensitive Wistar–Kyoto, but not Wistar, rats. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2011;35:67–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Hellman K, Abel T, Fear Conditioning Increases NREM. Sleep. Behavioral neuroscience. 2007;121:310. https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC2947315/

  53. Hong J, Choi K, Fuccillo MV, Chung S, Weber F. Infralimbic activity during REM sleep facilitates fear extinction memory. Curr Biol. 2024;34:2247–2255.e5. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/38714199/

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are thankful to all members of the Daisuke Miyamoto laboratory for their invaluable assistance at every stage of the experiments. We also thank all members of Dr. Kaoru Inokuchi laboratory for We are grateful to Dr. Shuntaro Sasai (Araya Inc. Tokyo, Japan) for valuable comments on statistical analysis.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from JSPS/MEXT KAKENHI (JP21H00305, JP21K15204); JST (JPMJFR204F); AMED (JP21wm0525009); the Takeda Science Foundation; the Naito Foundation; the Nakajima Foundation; the Mitsubishi Foundation; the Astellas Foundation for Research on Metabolic Disorders; the Kao Foundation for Arts and Sciences; the Chugai Foundation for Innovative Drug Discovery Science; the Toyama Daiichi Scholarship Foundation; the Brain Science Foundation; the Hokugin Grant-in-Aid for Young Scientists; the Tamura Science and Technology Foundation to DM.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

DM: Conceptualization; Data curation; Formal analysis; Funding acquisition; Investigation; Methodology; Project administration; Resources; Software; Supervision; Validation; Visualization; Writing - original draft. MAM: Data curation; Investigation; Methodology; Resources; Software; Validation; Visualization; Writing - review & editing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daisuke Miyamoto.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All animal care and experiments were approved by the Animal Experiment Committee in University of Toyama.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The author reports no declarations of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Miyamoto, D., Mahmoud, M.A. Post-conditioning sleep deprivation facilitates delay and trace fear memory extinction. Mol Brain 17, 90 (2024). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13041-024-01163-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13041-024-01163-w

Keywords